Vipul Bhrigu,Toledo大学的博士,到Univ of Michigan Comprehensive Cancer
Center做博后。从09年12月开始“细致而系统”的暗中破坏同组博士女生Heather Ames
的样品。
女博士渐渐发现了问题,开始还以为是自己犯了错误,采取了一些措施防止出错,
甚至拿到未婚夫的实验室去做,总是有各种各样的问题。怀疑有人破坏,说给朋友,导
师,学校相关部门听,都劝她是不是自己多虑了,甚至认为是她自己工作不顺利,试图
归结于其他原因。
直到出现media上被洒酒精,并且接二连三的发生,终于正式由警方介入。女博士
被审问两次,测谎一次,才给实验室安装了两个摄像头,并且非常震惊(对于他们,对
于我肯定是理所当然)的发现实验室新来的,“和蔼可亲,友好,健谈”的印度博后
Vipul Bhrigu从冰柜中取出自己的样品,然后拿起擦桌子消毒用的酒精spray往里面一
阵乱喷。
庭审判了8.8K的罚款和6个月probation,40小时社区服务,给实验室设备、研究进
度、人员工资造成的损失要开听证会确定,初步数字72K。Vipul Bhrigu坦承是为了减
慢同事的进度让自己看上去好一点。当他的博后老板给以前的博士老板打电话时再次惊
人的发现,Vipul Bhrigu4月认罪,6月又回到Toledo大学做了博后,并谎称离开
Michigan的原因是与新老板不和,终于被再次解雇。
It is sentencing day at Washtenaw County Courthouse, a drab structure of
stained grey stone and tinted glass a few blocks from the main campus of
the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor. Judge Elizabeth Pollard Hines has
doled out probation and fines for drunk and disorderly conduct, shoplifting
and other mundane crimes on this warm July morning. But one case, number 10-
0596, is still waiting. Vipul Bhrigu, a former postdoc at the university's
Comprehensive Cancer Center, wears a dark-blue three-buttoned suit and a
pinched expression as he cups his pregnant wife's hand in both of his. When
Pollard Hines calls Bhrigu's case to order, she has stern words for him: "I
was inclined to send you to jail when I came out here this morning."
Bhrigu, over the course of several months at Michigan, had meticulously
and systematically sabotaged the work of Heather Ames, a graduate student in
his lab, by tampering with her experiments and poisoning her cell-culture
media. Captured on hidden camera, Bhrigu confessed to university police in
April and pleaded guilty to malicious destruction of personal property, a
misdemeanour that apparently usually involves cars: in the spaces for make
and model on the police report, the arresting officer wrote "lab research"
and "cells". Bhrigu has said on multiple occasions that he was compelled by
"internal pressure" and had hoped to slow down Ames's work. Speaking earlier
this month, he was contrite. "It was a complete lack of moral judgement on
my part," he said.
Bhrigu's actions are surprising, but probably not unique. There are few
firm numbers showing the prevalence of research sabotage, but conversations
with graduate students, postdocs and research-misconduct experts suggest
that such misdeeds occur elsewhere, and that most go unreported or unpoliced
. In this case, the episode set back research, wasted potentially tens of
thousands of dollars and terrorized a young student. More broadly, acts such
as Bhrigu's — along with more subtle actions to hold back or derail
colleagues' work — have a toxic effect on science and scientists. They are
an affront to the implicit trust between scientists that is necessary for
research endeavours to exist and thrive.
Despite all this, there is little to prevent perpetrators re-entering
science. In the United States, federal bodies that provide research funding
have limited ability and inclination to take action in sabotage cases
because they aren't interpreted as fitting the federal definition of
research misconduct, which is limited to plagiarism, fabrication and
falsification of research data. In Bhrigu's case, administrators at the
University of Michigan worked with police to investigate, thanks in part to
the persistence of Ames and her supervisor, Theo Ross.
"The question is, how many universities have such procedures in place
that scientists can go and get that kind of support?" says Christine Boesz,
former inspector-general for the US National Science Foundation in Arlington
, Virginia, and now a consultant on scientific accountability. "Most
universities I was familiar with would not necessarily be so responsive."
First suspicions
Ames, an MD PhD student, first noticed a problem with her research on 12
December 2009. As part of a study on the epidermal growth factor receptor,
a protein involved in some cancers, she was running a western blot assay to
confirm the presence of proteins in a sample. It was a routine protocol. But
when she looked at the blot, four of her six samples seemed to be out of
order — the pattern of bands that she expected to see in one lane appeared
in another. Five days later, it happened again. "I thought, technically it
could have been my mistake, but it was weird that they had gone wrong in
exactly the same way," says Ames. The only explanation, she reasoned, was
that the labelled lids for her cell cultures had been swapped, and she
immediately wondered whether someone was sabotaging her work. To be safe,
she devised a workaround: writing directly on the bottoms of the culture
dishes so that the lids could not be switched.
Next, Ames started having an issue with the western blots themselves.
She saw an additional protein in the sample lanes, showing that an extra
antibody was staining the blot. Once again, it could have been a mistake,
but it happened twice. "I started going over to my fiancé's lab and running
blots overnight there," she says. As the problems mounted, Ames was getting
agitated. She was certain that someone was monkeying with her experiments,
but she had no proof and no suspect. Her close friends suggested that she
was being paranoid.
Some labs are known to be hyper-competitive, with principal
investigators pitting postdocs against each other. But Ross's lab is a small
, collegial place. At the time that Ames was noticing problems, it housed
just one other graduate student, a few undergraduates doing projects, and
the lab manager, Katherine Oravecz-Wilson, a nine-year veteran of the lab
whom Ross calls her "eyes and ears". And then there was Bhrigu, an amiable
postdoc who had joined the lab in April 2009.
Bhrigu had come to the United States from India in 2003, and completed
his PhD at the University of Toledo, Ohio, under cancer biologist James
Trempe. "He was an average student," says Trempe. "I wouldn't say that he
was a star in the lab, but there was nothing that would make me question the
work that he did." Ross thought Bhrigu would be a good fit with her lab —
friendly, talkative, up on current trends in the field. Ames says that she
liked Bhrigu and at the time had little reason to suspect him. "He was one
of the last people I would have suspected didn't like me," she says.
On Sunday 28 February 2010, Ames encountered what she thought was
another attempt to sabotage her work. She was replacing the media on her
cells and immediately noticed that something wasn't right. The cells were "
just dripping off the plate", as if they'd been hit with something caustic.
She pulled the bottle of medium out from the fume hood and looked at it.
Translucent ripples, like those that appear when adding water to whisky,
were visible in the dark red medium. When she sniffed it, the smell of
alcohol was overpowering. This, she thought, was the proof she needed. "It
was clearly not my mistake," says Ames.
She fired off an e-mail to Ross. "I just found pretty convincing
evidence that somebody is trying to sabotage my experiments," she wrote.
Ross came and sniffed the medium too. She agreed that it didn't smell right,
but she didn't know what to think.
Lab investigation
Some people whom Ross consulted with tried to convince her that Ames was
hitting a rough patch in her work and looking for someone else to blame.
But Ames was persistent, so Ross took the matter to the university's office
of regulatory affairs, which advises on a wide variety of rules and
regulations pertaining to research and clinical care. Ray Hutchinson,
associate dean of the office, and Patricia Ward, its director, had never
dealt with anything like it before. After several meetings and two more
instances of alcohol in the media, Ward contacted the department of public
safety — the university's police force — on 9 March. They immediately
launched an investigation — into Ames herself. She endured two
interrogations and a lie-detector test before investigators decided to look
elsewhere.
At 4:00 a.m. on Sunday 18 April, officers installed two cameras in the
lab: one in the cold room where Ames's blots had been contaminated, and one
above the refrigerator where she stored her media. Ames came in that day and
worked until 5:00 p.m. On Monday morning at around 10:15, she found that
her medium had been spiked again. When Ross reviewed the tapes of the
intervening hours with Richard Zavala, the officer assigned to the case, she
says that her heart sank. Bhrigu entered the lab at 9:00 a.m. on Monday and
pulled out the culture media that he would use for the day. He then
returned to the fridge with a spray bottle of ethanol, usually used to
sterilize lab benches. With his back to the camera, he rummaged through the
fridge for 46 seconds. Ross couldn't be sure what he was doing, but it didn'
t look good.
Zavala escorted Bhrigu to the campus police department for questioning.
When he told Bhrigu about the cameras in the lab, the postdoc asked for a
drink of water and then confessed. He said that he had been sabotaging Ames'
s work since February. (He denies involvement in the December and January
incidents.)
Motives for misconduct
Misbehaviour in science is nothing new — but its frequency is difficult
to measure. Daniele Fanelli at the University of Edinburgh, UK, who studies
research misconduct, says that overtly malicious offences such as Bhrigu's
are probably infrequent, but other forms of indecency and sabotage are
likely to be more common. "A lot more would be the kind of thing you couldn'
t capture on camera," he says. Vindictive peer review, dishonest reference
letters and withholding key aspects of protocols from colleagues or
competitors can do just as much to derail a career or a research project as
vandalizing experiments. These are just a few of the questionable practices
that seem quite widespread in science, but are not technically considered
misconduct. In a meta-analysis of misconduct surveys, published last year (D
. Fanelli PLoS ONE 4, e5738; 2009), Fanelli found that up to one-third of
scientists admit to offences that fall into this grey area, and up to 70%
say that they have observed them.
Some say that the structure of the scientific enterprise is to blame.
The big rewards — tenured positions, grants, papers in stellar journals —
are won through competition. To get ahead, researchers need only be better
than those they are competing with. That ethos, says Brian Martinson, a
sociologist at HealthPartners Research Foundation in Minneapolis, Minnesota,
can lead to sabotage. He and others have suggested that universities and
funders need to acknowledge the pressures in the research system and try to
ease them by means of education and rehabilitation, rather than simply
punishing perpetrators after the fact.
But did rivalry drive Bhrigu? He and Ames were collaborating on one of
their projects, but they were not in direct competition. Chiron Graves, a
former graduate student in Ross's lab who helped Bhrigu learn techniques,
says that Ross is passionate but didn't put undue stress on her personnel. "
The pressures that exist in the system as a whole are somewhat relieved in
Theo's lab," says Graves, now an assistant professor running a teacher-
education programme at Eastern Michigan University in Ypsilanti. "Her take
was to do good science."
Bhrigu says that he felt pressure in moving from the small college at
Toledo to the much bigger one in Michigan. He says that some criticisms he
received from Ross about his incomplete training and his work habits
frustrated him, but he doesn't blame his actions on that. "In any kind of
workplace there is bound to be some pressure," he says. "I just got jealous
of others moving ahead and I wanted to slow them down."
Crime and punishment
At Washtenaw County Courthouse in July, having reviewed the case files,
Pollard Hines delivered Bhrigu's sentence. She ordered him to pay around US$
8,800 for reagents and experimental materials, plus $600 in court fees and
fines — and to serve six months' probation, perform 40 hours of community
service and undergo a psychiatric evaluation.
But the threat of a worse sentence hung over Bhrigu's head. At the
request of the prosecutor, Ross had prepared a more detailed list of damages
, including Bhrigu's entire salary, half of Ames's, six months' salary for a
technician to help Ames get back up to speed, and a quarter of the lab's
reagents. The court arrived at a possible figure of $72,000, with the final
amount to be decided upon at a restitution hearing in September.
Before that hearing could take place, however, Bhrigu and his wife left
the country for India. Bhrigu says his visa was contingent upon having a job
. A new hearing has been scheduled for October in which the case for
restitution will be heard alongside arguments that Bhrigu has violated his
probation.
Ross, though, is happy that the ordeal is largely over. For the month-
and-a-half of the investigation, she became reluctant to take on new
students or to hire personnel. She says she considered packing up her
research programme. She even questioned her own sanity, worrying that she
was the one sabotaging Ames's work via "an alternate personality". Ross now
wonders if she was too trusting, and urges other lab heads to "realize that
the whole spectrum of humanity is in your lab. So, when someone complains to
you, take it seriously."
She also urges others to speak up when wrongdoing is discovered. After
Bhrigu pleaded guilty in June, Ross called Trempe at the University of
Toledo. He was shocked, of course, and for more than one reason. His
department at Toledo had actually re-hired Bhrigu. Bhrigu says that he lied
about the reason he left Michigan, blaming it on disagreements with Ross.
Toledo let Bhrigu go in July, not long after Ross's call.
Now that Bhrigu is in India, there is little to prevent him from getting
back into science. And even if he were in the United States, there wouldn't
be much to stop him. The National Institutes of Health in Bethesda,
Maryland, through its Office of Research Integrity, will sometimes bar an
individual from receiving federal research funds for a time if they are
found guilty of misconduct. But Bhigru probably won't face that prospect
because his actions don't fit the federal definition of misconduct, a
situation Ross finds strange. "All scientists will tell you that it's
scientific misconduct because it's tampering with data," she says.
Still, more immediate concerns are keeping Ross busy. Bhrigu was in her
lab for about a year, and everything he did will have to be repeated.
Reagents that he used have been double-checked or thrown away. Ames says her
work was set back five or six months, but she expects to finish her PhD in
the spring.
For her part, Ames says that the experience shook her trust in her
chosen profession. "I did have doubts about continuing with science. It hurt
my idea of science as a community that works together, builds upon each
other's work and collaborates." Nevertheless, she has begun to use her
experience to help teach others, and has given a seminar about the
experience, with Ross, to new graduate students. She says that the
assistance she got from Ross and others helped her cope with the ordeal.
"It did help restore the trust," she says. "In a sense I was lucky that
we could catch it."